33
Copyright © 2000 Institute of Applied and Behavioral Management. All Rights Reserved.
Measuring Emotional Intelligence:
Development and Validation of an Instrument
Kerry D. Carson
Paula Phillips Carson
Betty J. Birkenmeier
University of Louisiana at Lafayette
ABSTRACT
Emotional intelligence has been discussed in the popular business press over the past few years
and has recently found its way into prestigious business and industrial psychology journals.
However, there is no validated instrument available in the academic literature for management
consultants, trainers, and business practitioners to use when surveying employees' perceptions of
their emotional intelligence. The authors therefore constructed and examined a new measure,
beginning with 269 items which were eventually trimmed to 30 items. All items loaded cleanly
on their respective factors. In Study 1 and Study 2, reliabilities were: Emotional Intelligence
measure = .86, 91; Empathetic Response dimension = .87, 92; Mood Regulation dimension =
.77, 76; Interpersonal Skills dimension = .82, 82; Internal Motivation dimension = .78, 81; and
Self-Awareness dimension = .69, .70, respectively. It was found that emotional intelligence was
positively related to proactive personality and personal control, but negatively related to
irritability/verbal hostility and emotional exhaustion. The construct's relationships with selected
demographic variables were inconclusive.
Measuring Emotional Intelligence:
Development and Validation of an Instrument
Emotional intelligence appears to be a concept which has now come of age. Touted as a
construct which can assist organizations in reducing turnover, identifying transformational
leaders, facilitating executive coaching, creating more effective work teams, improving
organizational culture, stimulating creativity, and enhancing employee acceptance of radical
change (Fernandez-Araoz, 1999; Huy, 1999), emotional intelligence is experiencing increasing
acceptance by organizational consultants and business practitioners working in domains as
varied as profit-seeking businesses, public school systems, and not-for-profit social service
agencies. The promised individual and organizational benefits yielded by a greater
understanding of emotional intelligence, coupled with the emerging assertion that everyone can
(and should) be trained to become more emotionally intelligent (Reich & Goleman, 1999), has
encouraged the emergence and/or strategic repositioning of consulting firms and institutions.
For example, Hay Group has joined the ranks of Daniel Goleman (1995, 1998; recognized
advocate and guru of emotional intelligence in the mainstream) and Case Western Reserve
University to sponsor a three-day seminar on emotional intelligence. And, numerous web sites
34
Copyright © 2000 Institute of Applied and Behavioral Management. All Rights Reserved.
have been established promoting greater awareness of the efficacy of emotional intelligence
measurement and enhancement (for example, see www.eq.org, www.eiconsortium.org, and
www.eqi.org).
It has been a decade since the construct of emotional intelligence was first introduced into
the psychology literature (e.g., Mayer, DiPaolo and Salovey, 1990; Mayer and Salovey, 1995;
Salovey and Mayer, 1989-90), it has more recently created a stir in the popular business press
(e.g., Bates, 1999; Epstein, 1999; Farnham, 1996; Koonce, 1996; Martinez, 1997; O'Brien,
1996), as well as in the academic business literature. In fact, emotional intelligence has been
discussed in such prestigious outlets as the Academy of Management Review (Huy, 1999), and
the Harvard Business Review, where Goleman (1998a) suggested that research "clearly shows
that emotional intelligence is the sine qua non of leadership" (p. 94). While there are many
proponents of emotional intelligence, caution is also warranted as some have suggested that
emotional intelligence is a very broad concept and is just another management fad (e.g.,
Tossman, 1999).
Despite all this press and publicity, what is evidently and frustratingly lacking is a
robustly validated, non-proprietary measure of emotional intelligence in the public domain.
Because it has been suggested by some that emotional intelligence may play a key role in the
workplace (Huy, 1999; Goleman, 1998b), it is important that there is a psychometrically-sound,
readily-available emotional intelligence instrument. No instrument has been published, however,
in the academic literature. Therefore, the purpose of this research effort is to develop a reliable,
valid measure of employees' perceptions of their emotional intelligence. Consultants,
practitioners, and trainers can use such a measure to assess their interventions to increase
emotional intelligence among employees, and researchers can assess the construct's importance.
Additionally, standardized measures of this type of intelligence can generate new research
streams (cf. House and Aditya, 1997).
Salovey and Mayer (1989-90) originally defined emotional intelligence as the "ability to
monitor one's own and others' feelings and emotions, to discriminate among them and use this
information to guide one's thinking and actions" (p. 189). Individuals high on emotional
intelligence defer immediate gratification and exhibit self-control in order to optimize pleasure
over their lifetime. Also, they display enlightened self-interest by engaging in activities that are
both pro-individual and pro-social. Finally, they neither over-repress nor over-express
emotionality, but rather feel emotions flexibly and appropriately to the situation (Mayer and
Salovey, 1995).
Daniel Goleman, author of the best-selling book titled Emotional Intelligence (1995) and
more recently Working with Emotional Intelligence (1998), indicates that emotional intelligence
is a multidimensional construct. He suggests that the five underlying factors of emotional
intelligence are: (a) empathetic response, the facility to pick up on others' feelings; (b) mood
regulation, the capacity to control negative emotions; (c) interpersonal skill, social competence to
interact smoothly with others; (d) internal motivation, the ability to delay gratification in pursuit
of a goal; and (e) self-awareness, psychological insight into one's own true feelings. These five
dimensions were used in the development of an emotional intelligence measure.
35
Copyright © 2000 Institute of Applied and Behavioral Management. All Rights Reserved.
Construct Validity
In attempting to determine the construct validity of a new measure, it is necessary to
examine the nomological network of the instrument by assessing its convergent and divergent
properties. To establish convergent validity, the emotional intelligence measure is compared to
two constructs: (a) proactive personality, and (b) personal control. Divergent validity is
evaluated by comparing the focal instrument to: (a) emotional exhaustion, and (b) irritability/
verbal hostility.
Those with a proactive personality look for opportunities, seek improvement, and want to
make a constructive difference. They enjoy overcoming obstacles and challenging the status
quo. They are goal-oriented, initiate change, and persevere until a task is accomplished
(Bateman & Crant, 1993). Proactive personalities are action-oriented individuals who, rather
than being constrained by a situation, look for ways to change, manipulate, and exploit the
environment (Seiber, Crant, & Kraimer, 1999). Similarly, individuals high on emotional
intelligence are adaptive. They understand how their behaviors contribute to life's outcomes, so
they actively engage in goal-setting and planning. They are willing to accept challenging tasks,
persist in the face of adversity, and reward themselves for their accomplishments (Goleman,
1995). Thus, high convergent validity would be expected between emotional intelligence and
proactivity.
Those individuals who are low on personal control like being told what to do and avoid
autonomy so they don=t get into trouble. They prefer that a supervisor dictate rather than
involve them in decision-making. They like the familiar and routine. In contrast, those high on
personal control are not passive and helpless when it comes to work. They feel a sense of
empowerment and want to take action. They persist in the face of obstacles (Frese, Kring,
Soose, & Zempel, 1996). Additionally, those high on personal control, like those high on
emotional intelligence, feel more connected and socially integrated at work (Ross & Wright,
1998). Emotionally intelligent individuals display initiative, strive for improvement, and show
flexibility while toiling through task performance (Goleman, 1998b). Emotionally intelligent
individuals are optimistic. They believe they can influence their environment, so they are self-
motivated to do so (Sosik & Megerian, 1999). They are self-confident, sure of their capabilities,
and want responsibility/control. Resultantly, they like being autonomous, making decisions, and
engaging in calculated risks (Goldman, 1998b; Salovey & Mayer, 1989-90). Given the
similarities between personal control and emotional intelligence, the constructs should be highly
correlated.
Emotional exhaustion, with the associated feelings of being over-extended and
psychologically drained, is the key component of burnout (Schutte, Toppinen, Kalimo, &
Schaufeli, 2000). Emotional exhaustion is the opposite of enthusiasm, commitment, energy,
spirit and creativity (Layman & Guyden, 2000). Individuals who feel emotionally exhausted are
typically drained, fatigued and stressed (Maslach & Jackson, 1984). Relatedly, those low in
emotional intelligence are likely to feel unrewarded, underappreciated, and distressed (Salovey
& Mayer, 1989-90). This negative affectivity translates into emotional exhaustion in the
workplace (cf. Staw, Bell & Clausen, 1986). In contrast, those scoring high on emotional
intelligence are able to challenge their foul moods and not dwell on negative thinking (Mayer &
36
Copyright © 2000 Institute of Applied and Behavioral Management. All Rights Reserved.
Salovey, 1995). Further, those high on emotional intelligence are even-tempered (Sosik &
Megerian, 1999), not irritable or verbally hostile. People who are irritable are impatient, lose
their tempers easily, are grouchy, and let unimportant irritations bother them. Behaviors
associated with verbal hostility include getting into arguments, using strong language, raising
voices, and saying disagreeable things (Richins, 1983). When anger is counterproductive in a
situation, and an individual continues to behave with irritability and hostility, low emotional
intelligence is being displayed (Mayer & Salovey, 1995). Therefore, there should be negative
relationships between irritability/verbal hostility and emotional intelligence and between
emotional exhaustion and emotional intelligence.
There may be some emotional intelligence differences between individuals based on
demographics. For example, Goleman (1995) suggests that emotional intelligence increases with
age. Additionally, there may be gender differences. For instance, women may have higher
levels of empathy (Trobst, Collins & Embree, 1994) and be better at encoding and decoding
nonverbal communication than men (Hall, 1987), thus implying a higher level of emotional
intelligence. Therefore, older individuals should report higher levels of emotional intelligence
than younger individuals and women should report higher levels of emotional intelligence than
men.
The Investigation
Construction of the emotional intelligence measure occurred in three major phases. In
the first stage of constructing the scale, one of the authors generated 269 positively and
negatively worded items to represent the full range of the five emotional intelligence dimensions.
Consistent with the theoretical underpinnings of extant research on emotional intelligence, the
basis of these items were derived directly from Goleman's (1995) conceptualization of the
emotional intelligence dimensions. Next, the second author assessed the items for clarity and
meaningfulness, sorting the items back into the five intended theoretical dimensions. This judge
trimmed all dimensions back to 16 items each, retaining the items that were evaluated to best
represent the dimension. In the second stage, a total of 80 items were then examined in Study 1
with undergraduate students to determine the factor structure and the reliabilities of the intended
instrument. In the final stage (Study 2), MBA student respondents were surveyed to examine the
emotional intelligence construct's nomological validity. Student samples were deemed
appropriate because of the independence of emotional intelligence and extensive work
experience (Goleman, 1998).
Study 1
The development of a multi-item measure involves not only item generation and content
validity verification, but also an assessment of the individual items and establishment of the new
measure's reliability (Churchill, 1979; Scarpello & Vandenberg, 1987). The items generated by
the authors should load cleanly on the five theoretical dimensions of emotional intelligence
(Hair, Anderson & Tatham, 1987). Additionally, items lowering internal consistency should be
identified and deleted. Nunnally (1978) suggests as a guideline that reliabilities should be above
a = .70.
37
Copyright © 2000 Institute of Applied and Behavioral Management. All Rights Reserved.
Method
Sample
To ensure the likelihood of sufficient score variation in Study 1, surveys were
administered to students in variety of business classes from the sophomore level through
foundation courses in the MBA program at a large regional university in the southeastern United
States. A total of 344 surveys were distributed during class periods. Five of these were not
sufficiently completed and were removed from the sample, leaving a total of 339 surveys.
Demographic information was collected on chronological age, gender, ethnic
background, and numbers of hours employed per week. The average age of the respondents was
24 years old, 51.5 percent were female, 77.1 percent were Caucasian while 12.4 percent were
African-American, and the average hours of employment per week were 20.5.
Procedures
The surveys were administered during class time in Study 1. It was emphasized that
completion of the survey was voluntary. Anonymity of the respondents was protected in that
names were not requested, and surveys were placed in an individual envelope upon completion.
The following prefatory instructions introduced the survey. "This survey includes statements for
measuring how you have felt and acted during your everyday encounters over the past few
months. It was prepared by university researchers to learn more about how a select group of
students generally feel about their life and work. Your responses will be completely
confidential. In no instance will an individual be identified as having provided a particular
response. The data will be used for research purposes only. This is not a test. There are no right
or wrong answers. The best answer is one that is honest. For each statement below, decide
which response best indicates your attitude or position - how much you agree or disagree with
the statement. Place the number of the response on the line at the left." The 80 items were
anchored with a five-point rating scale ranging from 1 for "Strongly Disagree" to 5 for "Strongly
Agree."
Results
Analyses were conducted on Study 1 data to examine the factor structure of the construct
and to determine the reliabilities of the overall measure and its dimensions. The pool of 80
emotional intelligence items was interpreted using principal-axes factor analysis with an
orthogonal rotation to a varimax criterion. The researchers extracted five factors, and items with
loadings greater than .40 were initially examined. Factor 1 was identified as Empathetic
Response (13 items); Factor 2 as Mood Regulation (15 items); Factor 3 as Interpersonal Skills
(13 items); Factor 4 as Internal Motivation (11 items); and Factor 5 as Self-Awareness (10
items). Given that multidimensional measures are ideally reduced to the same number of items
per factor and that long measures can result in response fatigue, the dimensions were tailored to
six items each (Rummel, 1970). All items with loadings at or above .30 on other dimensions
were eliminated and the highest remaining loadings on the respective dimensions were chosen.
The retained 30 items and their loadings from this initial extraction are reported in Table 1.
38
Copyright © 2000 Institute of Applied and Behavioral Management. All Rights Reserved.
39
Copyright © 2000 Institute of Applied and Behavioral Management. All Rights Reserved.
An examination of the internal consistency and domain sampling adequacy suggested
that these items sufficiently tapped the factor structure of emotional intelligence. Reliability for
the overall measure was .86, and the reliabilities for the Empathetic Response, Mood Regulation,
Interpersonal Skills, Internal Motivation, and Self-Awareness dimensions were .87, .77, .82, .76,
and .69, respectively. Thus, the overall measure and its dimensions are adequately reliable,
except for a lower than desired internal consistency of the Self-Awareness dimension.
Correlations among the dimensions as well as with demographic variable are shown in Table 2.
Study 2
To establish the construct validity of the self-report measure, Study 2 was conducted to
test the relationships of the overall instrument and its dimensions with other established
constructs.
40
Copyright © 2000 Institute of Applied and Behavioral Management. All Rights Reserved.
Sample
Ninety MBA students at the large regional southeastern university completed surveys in
Study 2 (100% response rate; 100% useable). Of these respondents, 43% were female, 49%
were married, and the average age was 30.
Procedures
All surveys were distributed during class periods, and the MBA sample received similar
instructions from the researchers as did the undergraduate students in Study 1. Directions for
Section A of the questionnaire were "The survey begins with statements about your PERSONAL
FEELINGS. For each statement below, decide which response best indicates your attitude or
position - how much you agree or disagree with the statement. Place the number of the response
to the left." A 5-point rating scale was used to measure responses (1 = strongly disagree to 5 =
strongly agree). For Section B, the instructions were "Below are a number of statements each of
which you may agree or disagree with depending on your own personal evaluation of YOUR
PRESENT JOB AND YOUR ROLE. If you are a full time student, you may consider school to be
your present job. Please indicate your agreement or disagreement with each statement." The
final section of the survey asked for demographic-related information.
Measures
The following measures were used in Study 2 to assess construct validity.
Emotional Intelligence was tapped in the MBA sample (a = .91) with the 30 items developed in
the first phase of this study. The 6-item dimensional reliabilities for the sample were: (a)
empathetic response (a = .92), (b) mood regulation (a = .76), (c) interpersonal skills (a = .82), (d)
internal motivation (a = .81), and (e) self-awareness (a = .70).
Proactive Personality Scale (a = .82) was gauged with seven items from the scale developed by
Bateman and Crant (1993). One sample item is "I am constantly on the lookout for new ways to
improve my life."
Irritability/Verbal Hostility (a = .70) was a seven-item measure from Richins (1983). Sample
items include "I lose my temper easily but get over it quickly." and "I can't help getting into
arguments when people disagree with me."
Emotional Exhaustion (a = .80) was a 3-item measure adapted from the Maslach and Jackson
(1984) burnout scale. One sample item is "I feel emotionally drained from my work."
Personal Control (a = .80) was gauged using four reversed scored items from Frese and
colleagues' (1996) control rejection instrument. A sample item is "I would rather be told exactly
what I have to do. Then I make fewer mistakes" (reversed scored).
41
Copyright © 2000 Institute of Applied and Behavioral Management. All Rights Reserved.
Results
For the MBA sample, convergent validity was supported as there was a positive
relationship between proactive personality and perceived emotional intelligence (r = .61, p < .01;
empathetic response, r = .37, p < .01; mood regulation, r = .38, p < .01; interpersonal skills, r =
.45, p < .01; internal motivation, r = .70, p < .01; self-awareness, r = .40, p < .01). Also, there
was a positive relationship between personal control and perceived emotional intelligence (r =
.51, p < .01; empathetic response, r = .37, p < .01; mood regulation, r = .28, p < .01;
interpersonal skills, r = .36, p < .01; internal motivation, r = .33, p < .01; self-awareness, r = .46,
p < .01).
Further, divergent validity was supported with a negative relationship between irritability/verbal
hostility and perceived emotional intelligence (r = -.53, p < .01; empathetic response, r = -.37, p
< .01; mood regulation, r = -.53, p < .01; interpersonal skills, r = -.32, p < .01; internal
motivation, r = -.27, p < .01; self-awareness, r = -.41, p < .01). Also, there was a negative
relationship between emotional exhaustion and perceived emotional intelligence (r = -.39, p <
.01) though the interpersonal skills dimension was nonsignificant (interpersonal skills, r = -.19,
ns; empathetic response, r = -.22, p < .05; mood regulation, r = -.37, p < .01; internal motivation,
r = -.33, p < .01; self-awareness, r = -.35, p < .01). See Table 3
In Study 2, the demographic relationships were not as expected for emotional intelligence
or any of its dimensions. Older individuals did not report significantly higher levels of emotional
42
Copyright © 2000 Institute of Applied and Behavioral Management. All Rights Reserved.
intelligence than younger ones (r = .16, ns) which may be due to limited variance in the sample.
Further, women did not report significantly higher levels than men (r = -.07, ns.
Discussion
In Study 1, items tapping the five factors of emotional intelligence were identified and
their psychometric properties examined. Factor analysis revealed 62 items that loaded cleanly on
their respective factors, and these were trimmed to six items for each of the five factors. The
overall measure was reliable (a = .86). All underlying dimensions displayed adequate reliability
except that the self-awareness dimension was slightly low (a = .69). Correlations between the
five dimensions ranged from .04 to .42.
For the MBA sample in Study 2, emotional intelligence was positively related to
proactive personality and personal control and was negatively correlated with irritability/verbal
hostility and emotional exhaustion. The age-emotional intelligence relationship was not
significant for the MBA group (r = .16, ns), nor was gender-emotional intelligence relationship (r
= -.07, ns). However, post-hoc analyses of data from the undergraduate students in Study 1
yielded linkages between two relevant demographic variables and the overall measure suggested:
(a) increasing levels of emotional intelligence as one ages, and (b) possible underlying
dimensional differences in emotional intelligence between male and female undergraduate
students. Older individuals reported higher levels of emotional intelligence than younger
individuals (r = .23, p < .01), primarily due to the self-awareness and interpersonal skills
dimensions (r = .23, p < .01; r = .17, p < .01, respectively). Women did not report higher levels
of emotional intelligence than men in this sample (r = -.09, ns). However, using one-way
analysis-of-variance, women were found to perceive themselves as having better interpersonal
skills (F = 3.92, p < .05) and more self-awareness (F = 14.88, p < .01) than men. Men reported
better regulation of moods than women at a marginal level of significance (F = 2.80, p < .10), but
there was no difference between the two groups on internal motivation (F = .22, ns) or
empathetic response (F = 1.84, ns). Because of the mixed and weak results, the relationships
between emotional intelligence and its dimensions with demographic variables seem to be
inconclusive.
While there is some variation among the results reported, these two studies appear to be
instrumental in providing initial information on the psychometric properties and construct
validity of a 30-item emotional intelligence measure. This measure should prove useful to
consultants and practitioners who want to tap employees' perceptions of their emotional
intelligence. Having this information can be of value as it appears that skills associated
emotional intelligence can be learned (Goleman, 1995, 1998b). However, it should be noted that
this is a self-reported measure of emotional intelligence. Therefore, some social issues (such as
impression management) or personality types (such as the narcissists) may influence the
respondents= answers. In addition, this instrument needs further external validation in
organizations where there are wide variations in emotional intelligence.
Increasing emotional intelligence in the workplace through training and development
efforts could help organizations in several ways. For example, workers with high emotional
intelligence will provide customers with more empathetic and respectful service (O'Brien, 1996).
43
Copyright © 2000 Institute of Applied and Behavioral Management. All Rights Reserved.
In addition, the performance of teams would be facilitated (Gibbs, 1995; Martinez, 1997).
Further, high emotional intelligence would translate into better collaborative relationships across
cultures (cf. Brown, 1994) and better leadership skills (Goleman, 1998a).
Once the trainer has successfully raised the emotional intelligence of employees,
managers may have to consider different control mechanisms. The exploratory studies examined
here generally suggest that those high on emotional intelligence do not like being given orders.
Further, the extant literature suggests that they prefer to be involved in establishing their own
objectives. They want to be creative and flexible, emphasizing goals that are both good for them
as individuals and good for others. Because those high on emotional intelligence are adaptable,
they would avoid being controlled through continuance organizational commitment, becoming
entrapped because of economic and sunk-cost issues. Further, they would likely become
affectively committed to an organization only if there was true reciprocity in the exchange.
Individuals high in emotional intelligence are self-starters with insight into their personal
decision-making processes. They are good at networking. These types of skills suggest that they
would be more committed to their careers rather than to their organizations (Carson & Carson,
1998). Because workers can no longer count on their organizations for long-term employment,
individuals with high emotional intelligence will likely have the appropriate skills to survive in
today's fluid business environment. They are capable of weighing their own options and
deciding on a career direction (Farnham, 1996; Koonce, 1996). Thus, emotional intelligence
appears to be an important construct for researchers interested in loyalty and career issues.
Emotional intelligence seems to be an emerging area of study for management
researchers, because it meets two important criteria. First, it is rational and, second, it is
progressive (Abrahamson, 1996). It is rational in that organizational performance can be
improved when workers' emotional intelligence is heightened. It is progressive in that it is a
somewhat optimistic and spiritual view of human nature that is grounded in science (cf. Brandt,
1996). By using the 30-item measure developed these study, researchers have an opportunity to
be on the forefront in this area of study, and trainers have the ability to assess the effectiveness of
their interventions.
44
Copyright © 2000 Institute of Applied and Behavioral Management. All Rights Reserved.
References
Abrahamson, E. (1996). Management fashion. Academy of Management Review, 21,
254-285.
Bateman, T. S., & Crant, J. M. (1993). The proactive component of organizational
behavior: A measure and correlates. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 14, 103-
118.
Bates, S. (1999). Your emotional skills can make or break you. Nation=s Business, 87(4),
17.
Brandt, E. (1996). Workplace trends: Corporate pioneers explore spirituality. HR
Magazine (Internet download), 1-5.
Brown, T. L. (1994, April 4). Conquering complexity. Industry Week, 19.
Carson, K. D., & Carson P. P. (1998). Career commitment, competencies, and
citizenship. Journal of Career Assessment, 6, 195-208.
Churchill, G. A., Jr. (1979). A paradigm for developing better measures of marketing
constructs. Journal of Marketing Research, 16, 64-73.
Epstein, R. (1999). The key to our emotions. Psychology Today, 32(4), 20.
Farnham, A. (1996, January 15). Are you smart enough to keep your job? Fortune, 133,
34-40.
Fernandez-Araoz, C. (1999, July-August). Emotional intelligence. Harvard Business
Review, 77(4), 108-124.
Frese, M., Kring, W., Soose, A., & Zempel, J. (1996). Personal initiative at work:
Differences between East and West Germany. Academy of Management Journal,
39, 37-63.
Gibbs, N. (1995, October 2). The EQ factor. Time, 60-67.
Goleman, D. (1998a, November-December). What makes a leader? Harvard Business
Review, 93-102.
Goleman, D. (1998b). Working with emotional intelligence. Bantam Books, New York.
Goleman, D. (1995). Emotional intelligence. New York: Bantam Books.
45
Copyright © 2000 Institute of Applied and Behavioral Management. All Rights Reserved.
Hall, J. A. (1987). On explaining gender differences: The case of nonverbal
communication. In P. Shaver & C. Hendrick (eds.), Sex and gender, volume 7:
Review of Personality and Social Psychology (pp. 177-200). Beverly Hills: Sage.
Hair, J. G., Jr., Anderson, R E., & Tatham, R. L. (1987). Multivariate data analysis (2nd
ed.). New York: Macmillan.
House, R. J., & Aditya, R. N. (1997). The social scientific study of leadership: Quo
vadis? Journal of Management, 23, 409-473.
Huy, Q. N. (1999). Emotional capacity, emotional intelligence, and radical change.
Academy of Management, 24, 325-345.
Koonce, R. (1996, February). Emotional IQ, a new secret of success? Training &
Development, 50, 19.
Layman, E. J., & Guyden, J. A. (2000). Renewal versus burnout: A career blueprint. The
Health Care Manager, 18(3), 52-62.
Martinez, M. N. (1997, November). The smarts that count. HRMagazine, 72-78.
Maslach, C., & Jackson, S. E. (1984). Patterns of burnout among a national example of
public contract workers. Journal of Health and Human Resources Management,
Spring, 189-212.
Mayer, J. D., DiPaolo, M., & Salovey, P. (1990). Perceiving affective content in
ambiguous visual stimuli: A component of emotional intelligence. Journal of
Personality Assessment, 54, 772-781.
Mayer, J. D., & Salovey, P. (1995). Emotional intelligence and the construction and
regulation of feelings. Applied & Preventive Psychology, 4, 197-208.
Nunnally, J. (1978). Psychometric theory (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw Hill.
O'Brien, M. (1996, April). Developing your `EQ'. HRFocus, 3.
Ree, M. J., & Earles, J. A. (1993). g is to psychology what carbon is to chemistry: A
reply to Sternberg and Wagner, McClelland and Calfeee. Current Directions in
Psychological Science, 1, 11-12.
Reich, R., & Goleman, D. (1999). Point-counterpoint. Training, 53(4), 26-31.
Richins, M. L. (1983). An analysis of consumer interaction styles in the marketplace.
Journal of Consumer Research, 10, 73-82.
46
Copyright © 2000 Institute of Applied and Behavioral Management. All Rights Reserved.
Ross, C. E., & Wright, M. P. (1998). Womens work, mens work, and the sense of
control. Work and Occupations, 25, 333-355.
Rummel, R. J. (1970). Applied factor analysis. Evanston, IL: Northwestern U.
Salovey, P., & Mayer, J. D. (1989-1990). Emotional intelligence. Imagination, Cognition
and Personality, 9(3), 185-211.
Scarpello, V. & Vandenberg, R. J. (1987). The satisfaction with my supervisor scale: Its
utility for research and practical applications. Journal of Management, 13, 447-
466.
Schutte, N., Toppinen, S., Kalimo, R., & Schaufeli, W. (2000). The factorial validity of
the Maslach burnout inventory--general survey (MBI-GS) across occupational
groups and nations. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 73,
53-66.
Sosik, J. J., & Megerian, L. E. (1999). Understanding leader emotional intelligence and
performance: The role of self-other agreement on transformational leadership
perceptions. Group & Organization Management, 24, 367-390.
Staw, B. M, Bell, N. E., & Clausen, J. A. (1986). The dispositional approach to job
attitudes: A lifetime longitudinal test. Administrative Science Quarterly, 31, 56-
77.
Sternberg, R. J. (1997). Managerial intelligence: Why IQ isn't enough. Journal of
Management, 23, 475-493.
Sternberg, R. J. (1988). The triarchic mind: A new theory of intelligence. New York:
Viking-Penguin.
Tossman, D. (1999). EQ--vogue or value? New Zealand Management, 46(5), 34-36.
Trobst, K. K., Collins, R. L., & Embree, J. M. (1994). The role of emotion in social
support provision: Gender, empathy and expressions of distress. Journal of Social
and Personal Relationships, 11, 45-62.